The Wiley Handbook on The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

The Wiley Handbook on The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Morgan Barense, Audrey Duarte

Language: English

Pages: 480

ISBN: 1118332598

Format: PDF / Kindle (mobi) / ePub


The Wiley Handbook on the Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory presents a comprehensive overview of the latest, cutting-edge neuroscience research being done relating to the study of human memory and cognition.

 

  • Features the analysis of original data using cutting edge methods in cognitive neuroscience research
  • Presents a conceptually accessible discussion of human memory research
  • Includes contributions from authors that represent a “who’s who” of human memory neuroscientists from the U.S. and abroad
  • Supplemented with a variety of excellent and accessible diagrams to enhance comprehension

 

Moral Landscape

Psycholinguistics 101

Free Will and Consciousness: How Might They Work?

The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions

We Are Our Brains: A Neurobiography of the Brain, from the Womb to Alzheimer's

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

rstb.2002.1102. Rugg, M.D., and Wilding, E.L. (2000). Retrieval processing and episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (3), 108–115. doi: 10.1016/S1364‐6613(00)01445‐5. Scimeca, J.M., and Badre, D. (2012). Striatal contributions to declarative memory retrieval. Neuron, 75 (3), 380–392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.014. Scoville, W.B., and Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20 (1), 11–21. doi:

the conscious experience of memory, which is independent of true “oldness” (Slotnick and Schacter, 2004). Others have suggested that such processing is likely to reflect successful retrieval of the general properties of originally studied items such as shape and color (Garoff‐Eaton, Slotnick, and Schacter, 2006) or those involving the semantic label or general category (e.g., fruit, bird) to which the item belongs. As such, common activity in semantic processing regions, such as left temporal

and Funahashi, 2002, 2004, 2007). Another example is a study designed to dissociate the focus of spatial attention from the focus of spatial memory that finds the majority of delay‐active ­neurons to track the former (Lebedev et al., 2004). (Limits of the specificity assumption will also factor importantly in the consideration of “reverse inference” in neuroimaging, which appears further along, in the section on Implications of MVPA for ROI‐based analyses.) Examples of failures of sensitivity

episodic memory: levels of processing effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of ­ Sciences of the USA, 91 (6), 2008–2011. Kellenbach, M.L., Brett, M., and Patterson, K. (2001). Large, colorful, or noisy? Attribute‐ and modality‐specific activations during retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledge. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 1 (3), 207–221. Kim, H. (2010). Dissociating the roles of the default‐mode, dorsal, and ventral networks in episodic memory retrieval.

subregions contribute to human memory function poses a unique challenge for cognitive neuroscience research. Individuals with MTL lesions typically have damage that affects several MTL subregions, spanning both hippocampus and surrounding MTL cortices. Even those individuals with restricted hippocampal lesions have damage to multiple hippocampal subregions. Therefore, while the neuropsychological study of MTL patients has taught us a great deal about the essential nature of the region for

Download sample

Download