Human Intelligence and Medical Illness: Assessing the Flynn Effect (The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality)

Human Intelligence and Medical Illness: Assessing the Flynn Effect (The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality)

Language: English

Pages: 218

ISBN: 1441981330

Format: PDF / Kindle (mobi) / ePub


As critics will note, psychometric tests are deeply flawed. Person-to-person differences in performance on a psychometric test are not informative about many things of great interest. An intelligence quotient (IQ) cannot characterize creativity or w- dom or artistic ability or other forms of specialized knowledge. An IQ test is simply an effort to assess an aptitude for success in the modern world, and individual scores do a mediocre job of predicting individual successes. In the early days of psychology, tests of intelligence were cobbled together with little thought as to validity; instead, the socially powerful sought to validate their power and the prominent to rationalize their success. In recent years, we have ob- ated many of the objections to IQ that were so forcefully noted by Stephen Jay Gould in The Mismeasure of Man. Nevertheless, IQ tests are still flawed and those flaws are hereby acknowledged in principle. Yet, in the analysis that follows, individual IQ test scores are not used; rather, average IQ scores are employed. In many cases – though not all – an average IQ is calculated from a truly enormous sample of people. The most common circ- stance for such large-scale IQ testing is an effort to systematically sample all men of a certain age, to assess their suitability for service in the military. Yet, it is useful and prudent to retain some degree of skepticism about the ability of IQ tests to measure individual aptitudes.

An Alchemy of Mind: The Marvel and Mystery of the Brain

The Neurology of Consciousness: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropathology

Neuroscience-based Cognitive Therapy: New Methods for Assessment, Treatment and Self-Regulation

Basic Vision: An Introduction to Visual Perception

Brain Renaissance: From Vesalius to Modern Neuroscience

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Brain, 123, 665–676.PubMedCrossRef 97. Leary, P. M., et al. (1999). Childhood secondary (symptomatic) epilepsy, seizure control, and intellectual handicap in a nontropical region of South Africa. Epilepsia, 40, 1110–1113.PubMedCrossRef 98. O’Leary, S. D., Burns, T. G., & Borden, K. A. (2006). Performance of children with epilepsy and normal age-matched controls on the WISC-III. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 173–180.PubMedCrossRef 99. Wardlaw, T., et al. (2004). Low birthweight: country,

ratio as 8.7 (i.e., more than the conservative 7.7 ratio calculated above). Although more than 20% of the American workforce is functionally illiterate (compared to about 10% illiteracy in Germany and Switzerland), Heckman argues that it still makes more sense to spend money on children (p. 1902):Investing in disadvantaged young children is a rare public policy initiative that promotes fairness and social justice and at the same time promotes productivity in the economy and in society at large.

1. Steen, R. G. (2007). The Evolving Brain: The Known and the Unknown. New York: Prometheus Books. 2. Klein, R. G. (2003). Whither the Neanderthals? Science, 299, 1525–1527.PubMed 3. Green, R. E., et al. (2006). Analysis of one million base pairs of Neanderthal DNA. Nature, 444, 330–336.PubMed 4. Bocherens, H., et al. (2005). Isotopic evidence for diet and subsistence pattern of the Saint- Cesaire I Neanderthal: Review and use of a multi-source mixing model. Journal of Human Evolution, 49,

with diminished cognitive function 4 to 7 years later in children in a northeast Brazilian shantytown. The American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 66, 590–593.PubMed 76. Berkman, D. S., et al. (2002). Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy on cognition in late childhood: a follow-up study. Lancet, 359, 564–571.PubMed 77. Njenga, S. M., et al. (2007). Chronic clinical manifestations related to Wucheria bancrofti infection in a highly endemic

increase in white matter volume, such that there are rapid changes in the relative proportion of gray matter to white matter until about age 25 [15]. Because the overall brain volume is stable by age 12 [7], this implies that any decrease in gray matter volume after age 12 must be offset by an increase in the volume of either white matter [15] or the cerebrospinal fluid around the brain. Whether the decrease in gray matter volume between age 20 and age 40 can be entirely explained by white matter

Download sample

Download